Motivation and emotion/Book/2024/Humiliation
What leads to it, what role does it play, and how can it be managed?
Overview
edit
The Abu Ghraib torture and prisoner abuse. In March 2004, U.S. military at Abu Ghraib prison in Iraq violated detainees' human rights, including physical and sexual abuse, torture, and rape. The human rights violation done by the U.S. military in Abu Ghraib prison abuse (see Figure 1)[1] deprived Iraq detainees fundamental entitlements to recognition as human beings. These rights are normalized by the rule of law, both internationally and domestically. It is publicly acknowledged that every individual has equal status in accessing human rights. This normative expectation was undermined by the U.S. Army through dehumanization of Iraq detainees, depriving them of public acknowledgement.
|
The term "humiliation" etymologically derives from the Latin humilatio, meaning the act of prostrating or being dragged in the ground, in the mud (humus) (Araya,2020). It is a highly intense emotion (Elison & Harter, 2007) that responds to public degradation of identity (Hartling & Luchetta, 1999) by the actions of others (Torres & Bergner, 2012).
The theoretical consensus on key features of humiliating event include: 1. relational and interactive orientation (Klein, 1991; Miller, 1988); 2. public witnesses (Fernández,2023); 3. a perception of identity degradation (Hartling & Luchetta, 1999; Torres & Bergner, 2012); 4. the public degradation being an injustice (Hartling and Luchetta,1999).
Empirically, Jackson (1999) identified three characteristics of humiliation: {{ic|Use a numbered list or bullet points as taught in Tutorial 02} 1. the exposure of 'individuals' 'shortcomings or wrongdoings; 2. intentionality and publicity of the exposure; 3. perception of the exposure as unjust.
This chapter defines a humiliating stimulus as the unjust public degradation of social actors' identity. From a cognitive perspective on emotion, the chapter discusses the causes and responses of humiliation and its roles, while Gross emotion regulation theory will be used to explore strategies for humiliation management.
Focus questions:
|
What is humiliation?
editResearch has often related shame, embarrassment and guilt with humiliation . This chapter compares these four emotions based on their cognitive aspects. Cognitive appraisal distinguishes one emotion from another (Smith & Ellsworth, 1985). The comparison is made using the following ten dimensions: valence, attention, agency/coping, depth, certainty, goal, legitimacy, self-concept and responsibility (Smith & Ellsworth, 1985) (see Table 1).
Table 1.
Comparison Between Four Emotions
Humiliation | Shame | Embarrassment | Guilt | |
---|---|---|---|---|
Valence | negative | negative | negative | negative |
Activation | fight/undo damage by others; or flight/self-protection | flight/self-protection | flight/appease | fight/undo damage by self |
Attentional activity | attention demanding | attention demanding | attention demanding | attention demanding |
Control agency/coping potential | high agency when anger;
low agency when fear |
low agency | low agency | high agency |
Depth | serious, profound | serious, profound | serious, profound | serious, profound |
Certainty | unpredictable | unpredictable | unpredictable | unpredictable |
Goal path obstacles/goal relevance | identity | self-worth | inappropriate social behaviours | unethical, immoral behaviours |
Legitimacy | injustice | deserved | deserved | deserved |
Norm/Self-concept compatibility | incompatible; self being devalued | incompatible; self being inferior and unworthy | incompatible; self making an unintended social mistake | incompatible; self's behaviours harming others |
Responsibility | external attribution/blame others | internal attribution/self-blame | internal attribution/self-blame | internal attribution/self-blame |
Humiliation shares a few similar cognitive dimensions with shame, embarrassment and guilt, yet differs in others regarding legitimacy, norm/self-concept compatibility and responsibility
.Conceptually, this chapter refers to humiliation as an intense self-conscious negative emotion arising from cognitive appraisal of unjust public degradation of identity (Hartling & Luchetta, 1999; Klein, 1991; Jackson, 1999) by other social actors.
The causes of humiliation
editHumiliation as an emotion begins with the cognitive appraisal of a stimulus (Lazarus, 1982), influenced by interaction between biological processes and cognitive process (Levenson, 2002), and functions as an adaptive mechanism in human life.
Biological process
editAccording to Lazarus (1968), biological mechanisms predispose an association between types of stimuli and the corresponding emotions. Evolutionarily, a biological mechanism activates during violations of relational need (Baumeister & Leary, 2017), a need fundamental to human motivations (Baumeister & Leary, 2017). Evolutionary adaptability causes individuals to constantly evaluate their relations with the environment (Lazarus, 1991), to prepare for relational opportunities and challenges. This engrained biological mechanism underpins a relational, social emotion, such as humiliation.
The prefrontal cortex (PFC) is activated in appraisal (Dixon et al., 2017), with dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) activation in appraisal of emotional value (Golkar et al., 2012) and ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC) for negative appraisal and social cognition (Hiser & Koenigs, 2018). The limbic system (LeDoux, 2000), particularly the amygdala, reacts to danger and threats (LeDoux, 2003), especially sensitive to socially learned value (Lieberman et al., 2005). The insular cortex processes social pain (Lamm & Singer, 2010).
Cognitive process
editCognitive appraisal involves sense of self, internalized social norms, evaluation of one's own behavior, projection and prediction of external evaluation of the self and its actions (Lewis, 1995). In humiliation, one has appraised the unjust public degradation of identity, which causes the self to be lowered in the eyes of public in a social-cultural context (Jonas et al., 2014). Appraisal determines the emotional reaction to humiliation and is modulated by social and individual variables (Smith & Ellsworth, 1985).
Socio-cultural variables
editThe antecedent occurs within a social-cultural system where values are taught and learnt. The value can be reflected by whether the antecedent represents a public degradation of normative social statues (Schutz, 2012). Societies can be categorized primarily as honour societies or dignity societies, based on how social status is distributed (Araya, 2020).
Honour societies feature complex, rigid hierarchies, unequally structuring social relations based on honor (Araya, 2020). Different castes, classes, gender, race, ages and other social characteristics are differed with ranks of honor (Araya, 2020). For example, Roman Emperor Valeria was captured by the Persian King Shapur I and used a footstool by the King. It was a humiliation for the emperor, as they were of equal rank; while a servant would not experience it as humiliation (see Figure 2[2]).
Dignity societies hold the ideology of treating all humans with equal value, an ideal since Enlightment (Araya, 2010). However, it is often undermined by daily actions, such as racisms and ageism. In dignity societies, unequal treatment based on social groups membership is considered humiliation (Honneth, 1995), whereas inequality is the norm in honour societies.
Mental and personality variables
editIndividual variabilities exist in appraisal of the antecedent. Some individuals may appraise as it humiliating, even though it is ordinary. Mental or personality disorders impact cognition bias in information processing and heighten emotional responses (Fitzpatrick, 2023).
In particular, interpersonal sensitivity, a factor featuring depression (Collazzoni et al., 2014) and narcissism (Roche et al., 2013), is highly correlated with humiliation (Jonas et al., 2014). People with depression easily perceive negative stimuli as humiliating (Collazzoni et al., 2014). Grandiose narcissism had increased negative affect to humiliation and less forgiveness, exhibited maladaptive response (Roche et al., 2013).
Four U.S. presidents participated in an experiment. In the first condition, they were instructed to jump the queue and reprimanded by a confederate. In the second condition, they were instructed to stay in the line but were falsely accused of jumping the queue.
|
|
Emotional responses and roles
editStudies that focus on emotional responses of humiliation are rare, and few have examined the functions of these responses in human life. Insights can be drawn from research findings relevant to the experience of humiliation.
Emotional responses of humiliation
editCognitive appraisal determinates emotional effects (Lazarus, 1991). The triggered emotional responses consists of physiological arousal, subjective feelings and behavioral expressions. They serve the human functioning in the environment.
Physiological arousal
editUnjust public devaluation of identity threats social-self, causing increased attention and cortical activities, more intense than happiness or anger (Dickerson,2004; Otten & Jonas, 2014). It also activates hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis, triggering fight-or-flight responses (Otten & Jonas 2014).
Social threat may cause a sense of social exclusion, leading to decreased cognitive functioning and difficulty to control impulse (Otten & Jonas, 2013), correlating with flight-or-flight responses. Ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC) may activate during negative emotion and social cognition (Hiser & Koenigs, 2018); however, research has not directly studied the relationship between vmPFC and humiliation .
Threats to social status can raise blood pressure (Scheepers & Ellemers, 2005), signaling a fight-or- flight response (Curtis & O'Keefe, 2002). Humiliation associated with social pain may also activate the insular cortex (Masten et al., 2011), though, this connection has not been examined.
Case study Christ faced humiliation in many forms: rejection by his people, shame for miracles, mockery by the Roman soldiers, and ridicule during crucifixion. |
|
Behavioral expressions
editPhysiological arousal in response to humiliating stimuli indicates the activation of fight-or-flight responses. The responding behaviors are aggression (fight) and avoidance (flight) (Fernández et al., 2015; Miller, 1993).
Aggression
editRevenge is a common response to humiliation, shown as school shooting (Harrison & Bowers, 2010; Harter et al., 2003; Leary et al., 2003), homicide (Hale, 1994; Lindner, 2006; Miller, 1993), war (Lindner, 2006), terrorism (Lindner, 2001) and genocide (Lindner, 2009).Retaliation may target the perpetrators or public (Harter et al., 2003). Social tensions and violence (Klein, 1991) reflect the fight response.
Avoidance
editSocial avoidance or withdrawal is a common response of humiliation (Harter et al., 2003; ) and can increase risk of depression (Brown et al., 1995; Farmer and McGuffin, 2003; Kendler et al., 2003), and other mental illness (Klein, 1991), such as Post-Traumatic Stress Disorder (PTSD), in severe cases, suicide (Torres & Bergner, 2010). These behaviors reflect the flight response.
Subjective feelings
editAggression signals an urge to remove obstacles, indicating anger (Berkowitz, 1990). Humiliation associated violence reflects this anger. However, angry individuals may refrain from acting out due to limited resources, environmental constrains, personal standard (Novaco, 1976) or fear (Stemmler, et al., 2001).
Avoidance or withdrawal indicates self-protection, which reflects an urge arising from fear (Maddux & Rogers, 1983). However, individuals may also fight back due to limited escaping routes (Blanchard & Blanchard, 1990).
Anger
editBlanchard and Pearson (2018) stated
anger arises from appraising obstacles to goals and disputed resources (Lerner & Tiedens, 2006). Identity and the associated social status are personal resources in many societies (Fiske et al., 2016), and being "right" or "rights" to oneself (Jones, 1999), essential for one's relationship with the environment (Hauge, 2007). Appraisal of these "resources" being taken away generates anger.Fear
editFear prominently arises from appraisal of physical threat (Blanchard &Pearson, 2018), associated with feeling of powerless. Humiliating situations may also involve physical threats, assaults, or torture (e.g., see Figure 1). Additionally, identity threat and social exclusion may be perceived as physically unsafe, due to the shared neural underpinnings of physical and social pain (Eisenberger, 2012). However, research has not examined these neural underpinnings in relation to humiliation.
Humiliation: the roles of adaptation
editEmotion itself is an adaptive mechanism for human survival in the environment (Lazarus, 1968). From evolutionary and cognitive appraisal perspective, humiliation serves as an adaptive mechanism.
The evolutionary perspective
editEvolutionary theory posits that emotional responses help overcome environmental challenges and enhance reproductive opportunities (Blanchard & Pearson, 2018), hence, the ability to experience emotions is adaptive (Nesse 1990). Emotions direct attention to environmental information (Yiend, 2010) and motivate survival behaviours (Buck, 1985). This evolutionary view applies particularly well in understanding negative emotions (Blanchard & Pearson, 2018, p151).
Survival
editThe act to remove obstacles to deprived social status or withdraw for self-protection reflect anger and fear. Both emotional responses are adaptive function that maximize survival chances.
Emotions shape perception and memory (Laird et al.,1982), as seen in fear conditioning (LeDoux, 1993; Öhman & Mineka 2001). Future cues—like specific places, smells, or people—may trigger similar feelings (LeDoux, 1994). Past experience of humiliation helps individuals learn threat cues, offering an evolutionary advantage for protection against future humiliation.
Individual-society relationship
editDarwin (1872/1998) noted the communicative functions of emotional expressions in conveying needs among social members. Lazarus (1968) viewed emotional responses as adaptive mechanisms in controlling the organism-environment relationship. Emotional responses of humiliation help individual inform a society that has thwarted their relational needs,
Rejection of this fundamental need to human motivation (Baumeister & Leary, 1995) leads to profound social pain (Miller, 1988).Perpetrators or the public may feel guilt upon recognizing the emotional expressions of the humiliated and wish to make amends (Tangney et al., 2007). For example, humiliation in Iraq drew global attention, leading to human rights improvements (Mokhtari, 2009). Reports of humiliation in medical settings prompted actions to improve patient experience (Svindsth et al., 2013), and reconciliation has taken place to address the humiliation of Indigenous Australians (McMillan & Rigney, 2020).
Cognitive appraisal and adaptation
editThe adaptive mechanisms of emotions in survival and individual-social relationships can become dysfunctional due to misinterpretation of stimuli or emotional intensity (Keltner & Kring, 1998). Accurate perception and regulating emotion intensity depend on cognitive appraisal (Smith & Ellsworth, 1985).
Primary appraisal
editIndividuals with ordinary mental states and personality traits exhibit typical a cognitive appraisal (Smith & Ellsworth, 1985) of humiliating stimuli, as these stimuli are socially normative and easily recognizable within the same sociocultural context (Lazarus, 1991; Smith & Lazarus, 1990).
Whereas individuals with depression and grandiose narcissism may have faulty perception and misinterpret normal stimuli as humiliating (Alberto et al., 2014; Roche et al., 2013)
. Their preexisting dysfunctional emotional system (Miller et al.,2011) render humiliation a maladaptive mechanism, similar to other emotions.Secondary appraisal
editSecondary appraisal and reappraisal influence emotional intensity (Lazarus & Folkman, 1984). Highly intense emotions override individuals' executive function, which impairs their ability to choose alternative responses or evaluate consequences. They are prone to violence, substance abuse, self-injurious behaviour (Keltner & Kring, 1998).
Humiliation, the strongest negative emotion (Lindner, 2003), serves as an adaptive mechanism, when associated anger and fighting are expressed appropriately in a given sociocultural context. However, if mass killing is appraised as the best coping strategy, anger and fight becomes maladaptive emotional responses.
Humiliation associated fear and avoidance may serve individuals as self-protection. However, if individuals cannot unpair their initial appraisal from the stimuli or responses, they may develop mental disorders. Fear and avoidance become maladaptive. Regulating humiliation intensity through cognitive reappraisal therefore is key to maintain its functions as adaptive mechanisms.
Case Study When Gandhi arrived in South Africa, he was denied seating with European passengers because of his skin color and heritage. He refused to comply and got beaten, kicked into a gutter, and thrown off a train for insisting on staying in first class. On another occasion, a police officer abruptly removed him from a public footpath. Finding this treatment humiliating and inhumane, Gandhi who was initially uninterested in politics, decided to fight for rights. |
- Quiz
Humiliation management
editCurrent research has not addressed humiliation management, focusing instead on solving it as a social, political, and cultural issue (Kramer, 2006). The management of humiliation as an emotion is discussed here through the lens of emotion regulation (Gross, 1998), using the process model of emotion regulation (Gross, 1998) as a framework.
What is humiliation management?
editThe adaptation mechanism of emotion can become maladaptive due to high intensity (Dixon-Gordon et al., 2015; Lazarus, 1968). Healthy individuals prefer using distraction to disengage from negative emotions (Sheppes et al., 2011). However, such flexibility can diminish. When disengagement fails (Kashdan & Rottenberg, 2010), individuals may adopt inappropriate coping strategies. The need to manage humiliation then arises for regulating emotional intensity.
Some research views regulating emotional intensity as simply "dampening" it (Aldao et al., 2010). This chapter argues that the goal is to optimize emotional adaptability through effective coping strategies. It defines humiliation management as the process of regulating humiliation intensity for optimal coping and adaptation.
Cognitive change: the process model of emotion regulation
editThe process model of emotion regulation is the most widely used framework for deriving emotion regulation strategies(Webb et al., 2012; Gross, 1998). It has five components: situation selection, situation modification, attention deployment, cognitive change, and response modulation (Webb et al., 2012; Gross, 1998). Cognitive change is the stage where individuals start to engage in processing emotional information that has not been filtered through the first three components. Cognitive reappraisal, a type of cognitive change, determines emotional intensity (Blanchard & Pearson, 2008). It focuses on changing the sensory input of the stimuli (Gross, 1998), through either primary appraisal (reappraising meaning) or secondary appraisal (reappraising coping capacity) (Lazarus & Folkman, 1984).
Cognitive reappraisal: the effectiveness of a bidirectional mechanism
editEffective cognitive reappraisal increases activation in lateral prefrontal regions associated with cognitive control, while simultaneously decreasing activation in the amygdala and medial orbitofrontal cortex (Ochsner et al., 2002). Buhle et al. (2014) found that decreased amygdala activity results from altered semantic representations of emotional stimuli in the prefrontal cortex, influenced by cognitive appraisal. Mcrea et al. (2012) found that when facing negative emotional stimuli, activation in amygdala and insula activities can be reduced effectively by cognitive appraisal, when the goal is to decrease negative affects instead of increasing positive affects.
Empirical research shows that cognitive reappraisal effectively regulate emotional intensity through a bidirectional mechanism (Ochsner et al., 2002), activating the prefrontal cortex while simultaneously deactivating amygdala. Mindfulness has been identified as a cognitive reappraisal technique for its similar function of activating the prefrontal cortex while simultaneously deactivating amygdala (Brown et al., 2003; Way et al., 2010).
Mindfulness: a bidirectional model
editMindfulness encompasses a broad range of concepts and techniques but is not inherently used as a bidirectional mechanism. Some studies used it as a top-down or bottom-up mechanism (Chiesa et al., 2013). This chapter suggests identifying specific techniques and their specific application that enable mindfulness to function as cognitive reappraisal--regulating emotional intensity through a bidirectional mechanism.
Defining mindfulness
editFour perspectives on defining mindfulness can be summarized from current research:
1 ) a set of techniques for non-evaluative observation (Liija et al., 2013); 2 ) a measurable mental ability to pay attention (Shaprio, 2009), using Brown and Ryan (2003) Mindfulness Attention Awareness Scale; 3) an act of seeking novel information relevant to the present situation (Langer, 2000); and 4) a set of techniques that induce relaxation response (Sevinc et al., 2018).These four perspectives highlight key qualities of mindfulness: non-evaluative observation, attentiveness, novelty-seeking, and relaxation. Accordingly, mindfulness can be defined as a process of non-evaluative observation during novelty seeking, with attentiveness and relaxation response.
A preliminary hypothesis
editNovel information captures attention (Escera et al., 1998) and activates amygdala (Blackford, 2010). However, the relaxation response mitigates the flight-or-flight response (Jacobs, 2001), while non-evaluative observation leads to a neutral interpretation of novel information (Hofmann et al., 2010). Novelty also stimulates cognitive function and context-appropriate problem-solving (Larger & Moldoveanu, 2000). Organisms with a reduced fight-or- flight response, can experience emotions while coping with situational-appropriate strategies.
An experiment can be designed to test the efficacy of mindfulness in managing humiliation. Participants will be in a mindfulness session incorporating techniques that implement the four key qualities: non-evaluative observation, attentiveness, novelty-seeking, and relaxation. During the session, participants will be exposed to a strong negative emotional stimulus, such as a humiliating event. Neural activities in the prefrontal cortex and amygdala will be measured.
The hypothesis is that the prefrontal cortex regions will activate, while the amygdala will deactivate simultaneously. No flight-or-flight response will be observed; instead, a relaxation response will occur. This may illustrate that mindfulness functions similarly to cognitive reappraisal in regulating emotional intensity through bidirectionally during humiliation management.
Conclusion
editThis chapter framed the antecedent of humiliation in the context of unjust public degradation of identity. Unlike other emotional stimuli, the antecedent of humiliation has a cognitive element tied to a socio-cultural frame of reference, indicating a breach of social agreement on the treatment each social agency deserves. Its unique cognitive appraisal causes emotional responses of humiliation and distinguishes it from other negative, self-conscious emotions, such as shame, guilt and embarrassment. However, cognitive appraisal shifts over time, allowing changes between emotions (Clore & Ortony, 2000). This raises the question for future research of whether multiple appraisals and discrete emotions can coexist at a single point of time of sensory input.
Emotional responses of humiliation include physiological arousal, behavioral expressions and subjective feelings. The cortical region and limbic system, particularly in areas linked to threat and stress, will activate, resulting in a fight-or-flight response. Correspondingly, aggression and avoidance are common behavioral expressions of humiliation, which may contribute to terrorism and mental disorders. Anger and fear were involved, reacting to the perceived injustice, threat and danger.
However, researchers mentioned shame, guilt or embarrassment may also emerge. Due to the limited time and scope, this chapter did not explore how shame, guilt or embarrassment, as discrete, self-conscious emotions, may also manifest as subjective feelings of humiliation.
The emotional responses of humiliation serve as adaptive mechanisms in humans' functioning, enhancing chances of survival, controlling in relations with the environment, and facilitating social communication. Cognitive appraisal determines perception and coping strategies, making emotions either adaptive or maladaptive.
The need to manage humiliation arises when emotion becomes maladaptive and results in inappropriate coping. Humiliation management aims to regulate emotional intensity through cognitive reappraisal at the stage of cognitive change in the process model of emotional regulation. This chapter specifically examined the potential of mindfulness as a cognitive appraisal strategy and its bidirectional effects on emotional regulation. A preliminary hypothesis was proposed for testing efficacy of mindfulness as a bidirectional model in humiliation management.
An additional perspective not covered in the chapter is that non-violent collective action, such as Gandhi's approach (Khanna, 2003), may be recognized as an alternative coping strategy to violence or avoidance, an outcome from humiliation management. Notably, Gandhi's non-violence and truthfulness in advocating for social status reflect two principles of yoga--Ahimsa and Satya, which has a close relationship with Buddhist mindfulness (Chandel, 2014).
See also
editExtreme emotions as motivation (Book chapter, 2015)
Humiliation (Wikipedia)
Public humiliation (Wikipedia)
Relational aggression (Wikipedia)
References
edit- ↑ "Abu Ghraib torture and prisoner abuse". Wikipedia. 2024-09-25. https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Abu_Ghraib_torture_and_prisoner_abuse#/media/File:Abu_Ghraib_23.jpg.
- ↑ "Valerian (emperor)". Wikipedia. 2024-09-09. https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Valerian_(emperor).
Araya, F. A. (2020). Humiliation. Social Anatomy of a dark emotion. Simmel Studies, 24(2), 91–117. https://doi.org/10.7202/1075568ar
Baumeister, R. F., & Leary, M. R. (1995). The need to belong: Desire for interpersonal attachments as a fundamental human motivation. Psychological Bulletin, 117(3), 497–529. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.117.3.497
Berkowitz, L. (1990). On the formation and regulation of anger and aggression: A cognitive-neoassociationistic analysis. The American Psychologist, 45(4), 494–503. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.45.4.494
Blackford, J. U., Buckholtz, J. W., Avery, S. N., & Zald, D. H. (2010). A unique role for the human amygdala in novelty detection. NeuroImage (Orlando, Fla.), 50(3), 1188–1193. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.12.083
Blanchard, D. C., & Blanchard, R. J. (2008). Defensive behaviors, fear, and anxiety. In Handbook of Behavioral Neuroscience (Vol. 17, pp. 63–79). Elsevier Science & Technology. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1569-7339(07)00005-7
Blanchard, D.C. & Pearson, A.H. (2018) In Fox, A. S., Lapate, R. C., Shackman, A. J., & Davidson, R. J. (Eds.), The nature of emotion: Fundamental questions (pp.151). Oxford University Press.
Brown, G. W., Harris, T. O., & Hepworth, C. (1995). Loss, humiliation and entrapment among women developing depression: a patient and non-patient comparison. Psychological Medicine, 25(1), 7–21. https://doi.org/10.1017/S003329170002804X
Brown, K. W., & Ryan, R. M. (2003). The benefits of being present: mindfulness and its role in psychological well-being. Journal of personality and social psychology, 84(4), 822.https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.84.4.822
Buck, R. (1985). Prime theory: An integrated view of motivation and emotion. Psychological Review, 92(3), 389–413. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.92.3.389
Buhle, J. T., Silvers, J. A., Wager, T. D., Lopez, R., Onyemekwu, C., Kober, H., Weber, J., & Ochsner, K. N. (2014). Cognitive reappraisal of emotion: a meta-analysis of human neuroimaging studies. Cerebral Cortex (New York, N.Y. 1991), 24(11), 2981–2990. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bht154
Clore, G. L., & Ortony, A. (1999). Cognition in emotion: Always, sometimes, or never? In Lane, R. D., & Nadel, L. (Eds.), Cognitive Neuroscience of Emotion (1st ed.) Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/oso/9780195118889.003.0003
Chandel, B. (2014). Gandhi on Non-Violence (Ahimsa). Diogenes (English Ed.), 61(3–4), 135–142. https://doi.org/10.1177/0392192116666470
Chiesa, A., Serretti, A., & Jakobsen, J. C. (2013). Mindfulness: Top–down or bottom–up emotion regulation strategy? Clinical Psychology Review, 33(1), 82–96. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2012.10.006
Collazzoni, A., Capanna, C., Bustini, M., Stratta, P., Ragusa, M., Marino, A., & Rossi, A. (2014). Humiliation and interpersonal sensitivity in depression. Journal of Affective Disorders, 167, 224–227. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2014.06.008
Curtis, B. M., & O’Keefe, J. H. (2002). Autonomic tone as a cardiovascular risk factor: The dangers of chronic fight or flight. Mayo Clinic Proceedings, 77(1), 45–54. https://doi.org/10.4065/77.1.45
Darwin, C., & Ekman, P. (1998). Expression of the emotions in man and animals (Corrected 3rd edition.). Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/oso/9780195112719.001.0001
Dickerson, S. S., Gruenewald, T. L., & Kemeny, M. E. (2004). When the social self is threatened: Shame, physiology, and health. Journal of Personality, 72(6), 1191–1216. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-6494.2004.00295.x
Dixon-Gordon, K. L., Aldao, A., & De Los Reyes, A. (2015). Emotion regulation in context: Examining the spontaneous use of strategies across emotional intensity and type of emotion. Personality and Individual Differences, 86, 271–276. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2015.06.011
Dixon, M. L., Thiruchselvam, R., Todd, R., & Christoff, K. (2017). Emotion and the prefrontal cortex: An integrative review. Psychological bulletin, 143(10), 1033-1081. https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000096
Eisenberger, N. I. (2012). The pain of social disconnection: Examining the shared neural underpinnings of physical and social pain. Nature Reviews. Neuroscience, 13(6), 421–434. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn3231
Elison, J., & Harter, S. (2007). Humiliation: Causes, correlates, and consequences. In J. L. Tracy, R. W. Robins, & J. P. Tangney (Eds.), The self-conscious emotions: Theory and research (pp. 310–329). The Guilford Press.
Elshout, M., Nelissen, R. M. A., & van Beest, I. (2017). Conceptualising humiliation. Cognition and Emotion, 31(8), 1581–1594. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2016.1249462
Escera, C., Alho, K., Winkler, I., & Näätänen, R. (1998). Neural mechanisms of involuntary attention to acoustic novelty and change. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 10(5), 590–604. https://doi.org/10.1162/089892998562997
Farmer, A. E., & McGuffin, P. (2003). Humiliation, loss and other types of life events and difficulties: a comparison of depressed subjects, healthy controls and their siblings. Psychological Medicine, 33(7), 1169–1175. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291703008419
Fernández, S., Saguy, T., Gaviria, E., Agudo, R., & Halperin, E. (2023). The role of witnesses in humiliation: Why does the presence of an audience facilitate humiliation among victims of devaluation? Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 49(1), 32-47. https://doi.org/10.1177/01461672211053078
Fiske, S. T., Dupree, C. H., Nicolas, G., & Swencionis, J. K. (2016). Status, power, and intergroup relations: The personal is the societal. Current Opinion in Psychology, 11, 44–48. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2016.05.012
Fitzpatrick, S., Dixon-Gordon, K. L., Turner, C. J., Chen, S. X., & Chapman, A. (2023). Emotion dysregulation in personality disorders. Current Psychiatry Reports, 25(5), 223–231. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11920-023-01418-8
Golkar, A., Lonsdorf, T. B., Olsson, A., Lindstrom, K. M., Berrebi, J., Fransson, P., Schalling, M., Ingvar, M., & Öhman, A. (2012). Distinct contributions of the dorsolateral prefrontal and orbitofrontal cortex during emotion regulation. PloS one, 7(11), e48107–e48107. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0048107
Gross, J. J. (1998). The emerging field of emotion regulation: An integrative review. Review of general psychology, 2(3), 271-299. https://doi.org/10.1037/1089-2680.2.3.271
Hauge, A. L. (2007). Identity and place: A critical comparison of three identity theories. Architectural Science Review, 50(1), 44–51. https://doi.org/10.3763/asre.2007.5007
Hariri, A. R., Tessitore, A., Mattay, V. S., Fera, F., & Weinberger, D. R. (2002). The amygdala response to emotional stimuli: A comparison of faces and scenes. NeuroImage (Orlando, Fla.), 17(1), 317–323. https://doi.org/10.1006/nimg.2002.1179
Hartling, L. M., & Luchetta, T. (1999). Humiliation: Assessing the impact of derision, degradation, and debasement. The Journal of Primary Prevention, 19(4), 259–278. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1022622422521
Hartling, L.M.,(2007). Humiliation: Real Pain, A Pathway to Violence. http://www.cchla.ufpb.br/rbse/HartleyArt.pdf
Hiser, J., & Koenigs, M. (2018). The multifaceted role of the ventromedial prefrontal cortex in emotion, decision making, social cognition, and psychopathology. Biological psychiatry, 83(8), 638–647. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2017.10.030
Hofmann, W., De Houwer, J., Perugini, M., Baeyens, F., & Crombez, G. (2010). Evaluative conditioning in humans: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 136(3), 390–421. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0018916
Honneth, A. (1995). The struggle for recognition: the moral grammar of social conflicts. Polity Press.
Jackson, M. A. (1999). Distinguishing shame and humiliation. ProQuest Dissertations & Theses.
Jacobs, G. D. (2001). The physiology of mind-body interactions: The stress response and the relaxation response. The Journal of Alternative and Complementary Medicine (New York, N.Y.), 7(1), S83–S92. https://doi.org/10.1089/107555301753393841
Jacobson, A. D. (2013). The role of humiliation in international conflict. Northern Plains Ethics Journal, 1(1), 66.
Jonas, K. J., Otten, M., & Doosje, B. (2014). Humiliation in conflict: Underlying processes and effects on human thought and behavior. In De Dreu & C. K. W (Eds.) Social Conflict Within and Between Groups (1st ed., pp. 37–54). Psychology Press. https://doi.org/10.4324/9781315772745-4
Jones, P. (1999). Human rights, group rights, and peoples’ Rrghts. Human Rights Quarterly, 21(1), 80–107. https://doi.org/10.1353/hrq.1999.0009
Kashdan, T. B., & Rottenberg, J. (2010). Psychological flexibility as a fundamental aspect of health. Clinical Psychology Review, 30(7), 865–878. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2010.03.001
Kendler, K. S., Hettema, J. M., Butera, F., Gardner, C. O., & Prescott, C. A. (2003). Life event dimensions of loss, humiliation, entrapment, and danger in the prediction of onsets of major depression and generalized anxiety. Archives of General Psychiatry, 60(8), 789–796. https://doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.60.8.789
Khanna. (2013). A narrative review of yoga and mindfulness as complementary therapies for addiction. Complementary Therapies in Medicine., 21(3). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ctim.2013.01.008
Klein, D. C. (1991). The humiliation dynamic: An overview. The Journal of Primary Prevention, 12(2), 93–121. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02015214
Lacey, D. (2011). The Role of Humiliation in Collective Political Violence. The University of Sydney.
Laird, J. D., Wagener, J. J., Halal, M., & Szegda, M. (1982). Remembering what you feel: Effects of emotion on memory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 42(4), 646–657. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.42.4.646
Lamm, C., & Singer, T. (2010). The role of anterior insular cortex in social emotions. Brain Structure and Function, 214(5–6), 579–591. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00429-010-0251-3
Langer, E. J., & Moldoveanu, M. (2000). The construct of mindfulness. Journal of Social Issues, 56(1), 1–9. https://doi.org/10.1111/0022-4537.00148
Lazarus, R. S. (1968). Emotions and adaptation: Conceptual and empirical relations. In Nebraska symposium on motivation. University of Nebraska Press.
Lazarus, R. S. (1982). Thoughts on the relations between emotion and cognition. The American Psychologist, 37(9), 1019–1024. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.37.9.1019
Lazarus, R. S. (1984). Stress, appraisal, and coping (Vol. 464). Springer.
Lazarus, R. S. (1991). Emotion And Adaptation (1st ed.). Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/oso/9780195069945.001.0001
LeDoux, J. E. (1993). Emotional memory systems in the brain. Behavioral Brain Research, 58(1), 69–79. https://doi.org/10.1016/0166-4328(93)90091-4
LeDoux, J. E. (2002). Emotion, memory and the brain. Scientific American, 12(1), 62–71. https://doi.org/10.1038/scientificamerican0402-62sp
LeDoux, J. E. (2000). Emotion circuits in the brain. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 23(1), 155–184. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.neuro.23.1.155
LeDoux, J. (2003). The emotional brain, fear, and the amygdala. Cellular and Molecular Neurobiology, 23(4–5), 727–738. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1025048802629
Lewis, M. D. (1995). Cognition-emotion feedback and the self-organization of developmental paths. Human Development, 38(2), 71-102. https://doi.org/10.1159/000278302
Levenson, R. W. (2002). Autonomic Specificity and Emotion. In Davidson, R. J., Scherer, K. R., & Goldsmith, H. H (Eds.) Handbook of Affective Sciences, 2, (pp.212-224). Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/oso/9780195126013.003.0011
Lieberman, M. D., Hariri, A., Jarcho, J. M., Eisenberger, N. I., & Bookheimer, S. Y. (2005). An fMRI investigation of race-related amygdala activity in African-American and Caucasian-American individuals. Nature Neuroscience, 8(6), 720–722. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn1465
Lilja, J. L., Lundh, L.-G., Josefsson, T., & Falkenström, F. (2013). Observing as an essential facet of mindfulness: A comparison of FFMQ patterns in meditating and non-meditating individuals. Mindfulness, 4(3), 203–212. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12671-012-0111-8
Lindner, E. G. (2001). Humiliation as the source of terrorism: A new paradigm. Peace Research, 33(2), 59–68.
Lindner, E. G. (2003). Humiliation or dignity: regional conflicts in the global village. The International Journal of Mental Health, Psychosocial Work and Counselling in Areas of Armed Conflict, 1(1), 48-63.
Lindner, E. G. (2006). Making Enemies: Humiliation and International Conflict (1st ed.). Praeger.
Maddux, J. E., & Rogers, R. W. (1983). Protection motivation and self-efficacy: A revised theory of fear appeals and attitude change. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 19(5), 469–479. https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-1031(83)90023-9
Masten, C. L., Morelli, S. A., & Eisenberger, N. I. (2011). An fMRI investigation of empathy for ‘social pain’ and subsequent prosocial behavior. NeuroImage (Orlando, Fla.), 55(1), 381–388. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.11.060
McMillan, M., & Rigney, S. (2018). Race, reconciliation, and justice in Australia: from denial to acknowledgment. Ethnic and Racial Studies, 41(4), 759–777. https://doi.org/10.1080/01419870.2017.1340653
McRae, K., Gross, J. J., Weber, J., Robertson, E. R., Sokol-Hessner, P., Ray, R. D., Gabrieli, J. D. E., & Ochsner, K. N. (2012). The development of emotion regulation: An fMRI study of cognitive reappraisal in children, adolescents and young adults. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 7(1), 11–22. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsr093
Miller, S. B. (1988). Humiliation and shame: Comparing two affect states as indicators of narcissistic stress. Bulletin of the Menninger Clinic, 52(1), 40–51.
Mokhtari, S. (2009). After Abu Ghraib: Exploring Human Rights in America and the Middle East (1st ed.). Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511581052
Nesse, R. M. (1990). Evolutionary explanations of emotions. Human Nature (Hawthorne, N.Y.), 1(3), 261–289. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02733986
Novaco, R. W. (1976). The functions and regulation of the arousal of anger. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 133(10), 1124–1128. https://doi.org/10.1176/ajp.133.10.1124
Otten, M., & Jonas, K. J. (2013). Out of the group, out of control? The brain responds to social exclusion with changes in cognitive control. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 8(7), 789–794. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nss071
Otten, M., & Jonas, K. J. (2014). Humiliation as an intense emotional experience: Evidence from the electro-encephalogram. Social Neuroscience, 9(1), 23–35. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470919.2013.855660
Ochsner, K. N., Bunge, S. A., Gross, J. J., & Gabrieli, J. D. (2002). Rethinking feelings: an FMRI study of the cognitive regulation of emotion. Journal of cognitive neuroscience, 14(8), 1215-1229. https://doi.org/10.1162/089892902760807212
Roche, M. J., Pincus, A. L., Conroy, D. E., Hyde, A. L., & Ram, N. (2013). Pathological Narcissism and Interpersonal Behavior in Daily Life. Personality Disorders, 4(4), 315–323. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0030798
Scheepers, D., & Ellemers, N. (2005). When the pressure is up: The assessment of social identity threat in low and high status groups. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 41(2), 192-200. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2004.06.002
Schutz, A., van Breda, H. L., & Natanson, M. A. (1972). Collected Papers I. the Problem of Social Reality (1st ed., Vol. 11). Springer Netherlands. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-010-2851-6
Sevinc, G., Hölzel, B. K., Hashmi, J., Greenberg, J., McCallister, A., Treadway, M., Schneider, M. L., Dusek, J. A., Carmody, J., & Lazar, S. W. (2018). Common and dissociable neural activity after mindfulness-based stress reduction and relaxation response programs. Psychosomatic Medicine, 80(5), 439–451. https://doi.org/10.1097/PSY.0000000000000590
Smith, C. A., & Ellsworth, P. C. (1985). Patterns of Cognitive Appraisal in Emotion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48(4), 813–838. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.48.4.813
Stemmler, G., Heldmann, M., Pauls, C. A., & Scherer, T. (2001). Constraints for emotion specificity in fear and anger: The context counts. Psychophysiology, 38(2), 275–291. https://doi.org/10.1111/1469-8986.3820275
Svindseth, M. F., Nøttestad, J.,A., & Dahl, A. A. (2013). Perceived humiliation during admission to a psychiatric emergency service and its relation to socio-demography and psychopathology. BMC Psychiatry, 13, 217. https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-244X-13-217
Tangney, J. P., Stuewig, J., & Mashek, D. J. (2007). Moral emotions and moral behavior. Annual Review of Psychology, 58(1), 345–372. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.56.091103.070145
Torres, W. J., & Bergner, R. M. (2012). Severe public humiliation: Its nature, consequences, and clinical treatment. Psychotherapy (Chicago, Ill.), 49(4), 492–501. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0029271
Trumbull, D. (2008). Humiliation: the trauma of disrespect. The Journal of the American Academy of Psychoanalysis and Dynamic Psychiatry, 36(4), 643-. https://doi.org/10.1521/jaap.2008.36.4.643
Way, B. M., Creswell, J. D., Eisenberger, N. I., & Lieberman, M. D. (2010). Dispositional mindfulness and depressive symptomatology: correlations with limbic and self-referential neural activity during rest. Emotion, 10(1), 12.https://doi.org/10.1037/a0018312
Yiend, J. (2010). The effects of emotion on attention: A review of attentional processing of emotional information. Cognition and Emotion, 24(1), 3–47. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699930903205698