Motivation and emotion/Book/2011/Sensation seeking

Sensation seeking:
How does it affect your life?

Introduction

edit

Focus questions

edit
 
  • What is sensation seeking?
  • Am I a sensation seeker?
  • How can an understanding of sensation seeking help me?

How can this page help me?

edit
  • Have you ever wondered why some people are attracted to intense, sometimes fear-inducing thrills while others recoil from them?
  • How can the same horror movie can be a torturous experience to one person and a favourite form of entertainment to another?
  • Is there something different going on inside the brains of these different people?

What is sensation seeking?

edit

Sensation seeking: “a trait describing the tendency to seek novel, varied, complex and intense sensations and experiences and the willingness to take risks for such experience” (Zuckerman, 1994, p. i).

The Sensation Seeking Scale (SSS) developed in the 1960s was the first operationalised measure of sensation seeking (Zuckerman, Kolin, Price, & Zoob, 1964). Since then, the theory behind the trait has developed with research.


Sensation seeking scores from the SSS have been linked to many risk-taking behavioural expressions. These include (Zuckerman, 1994):

  • driving habits
  • health
  • gambling
  • financial activities
  • alcohol and drug use
  • sexual behaviour and
  • sports

Sensation seeking is also involved in (Zuckerman, 1994):

  • vocational preferences and job choices
  • job satisfaction
  • relationships
  • eating habits and food preferences
  • media and art preferences
  • humour
  • fantasy and creativity and
  • social attitudes

Understanding one’s own and others’ tendency towards sensation seeking can help in all of these areas. This chapter will give an overview of the research on each of these aspects and summarise the practical implications of the research.

Measuring sensation seeking

edit

The Sensation Seeking Scale (SSS) is a self-report questionnaire developed by Zuckerman (1971). The scale measures the presence of four dimensions of sensation seeking in the participant including:

  1. disinhibition
  2. boredom susceptibility
  3. thrill and adventure seeking
  4. experience seeking

The SSS gives an overall accumulative score for each of the four dimensions. The scores then rank the behavioural tendencies of the participant, with low scores (LSS) and higher scores (HSS; Walker & Broughton, 2009).

Am I a sensation seeker?

edit

Complete this questionnaire to test your tendency towards varied, novel and intense sensations. This questionnaire is based on the SSS designed by Zuckerman (1971). Please chose either the answer that best describes yourself or mark the choice that you dislike the least.

NOTE: Answer the questions from each section before selecting 'submit'.

Section 1: Disinhibition

The desire for social and sexual disinhibition as expressed in social drinking, partying, and variety in sexual partners.

1

I like “wild” uninhibited parties
I prefer quiet parties with good conversation

2

I dislike people who are uninhibited and free about sex
I enjoy the company of real "swingers"

3

I find that stimulants make me uncomfortable
I often like to get high (use drugs)


Section 2: Boredom Susceptibility

Aversion to repetition, routine, and dull people, and restlessness when things are unchanging.

1

There are some movies I enjoy seeing a multiple times
I can’t stand watching a movie that I’ve seen before

2

I prefer friends who are excitingly unpredictable
I prefer friends who are reliable

3

I enjoy spending time in the familiar surroundings of home
I get very restless if I have to stay around home


Section 3: Thrill and Adventure Seeking

Desire to engage in sports or other activities involving speed or danger.

1

A sensible person avoids activities that are dangerous
I sometimes like to do things that are frightening

2

I would like to try surfboard riding
I would not like to try surfboard riding

3

I would not like to learn to fly an airplane
I would like to learn to fly an airplane


Section 4: Experience Seeking

The seeking of experiences through the mind and senses, travel, and a nonconforming life-style.

1

I don't mind getting lost
I prefer a guide when I am in a place I don’t know well

2

I prefer the “down to earth” kinds of people as friends
I would like to make friends in some of the “far-out” groups

3

I like to try new foods
I order the dishes with which I am familiar


Add the scores from each section together to get your overall sensation-seeking score.

The development of sensation seeking theory

edit

The goal of sensation seeking behaviour is to increase stimulation. Several different approaches have attempted to provide an explanation for this behaviour.

Instinct, drive and need approaches

edit

Freud (1915/1957) grouped instincts into two categories:

  • those serving life (e.g. sex, hunger, thirst and pain avoidance); and
  • those serving death (an expression of a conscious/unconscious wish to die)

as the ultimate aim. These death needs, which include sensation seeking, were regarded as displacements or sublimations. Finally, Freud also explained them as an attempt to deny fear by building tensions in order to increase pleasure with tension reduction.

Hull’s (1943) Drive Theory postulates that behaviour is driven in order to met a person’s physiological needs. It states that drive is the psychological impetus that energises, directs and motivates behaviour in order to satisfy physiological needs. Internal states of deprivation are deemed unpleasant and any behaviour that reduces unpleasant feelings associated with deprivation will be learnt and repeated. Drive Theory specified that behaviour could be predicted because a person would be motivated to engage in that behaviour which satisfied physiological needs (Deckers, 2005).

Following Drive Theory, Murray (1938) classified personality traits by the needs underlying them. Murray regarded all needs to have their origins in "tensions in the brain". Types of needs associated with sensation seeking include sex, sentience (need for sensation), exhibitionism and play. Other relevant needs relate to broad cognitive styles including change or sameness, impulsion or deliberation, and conjunctivity (organised response) or disjunctivity (disorganised response) (Zuckerman, 1994).

However:

  • Sensation seeking behaviour often arises from a state of low arousal and it is an increase in arousal, not a reduction in arousal, that is the goal state. This pattern is opposite that of other primary drives.
  • Maslow’s (1954) self-actualisation theory also challenged these theories with the idea that mature needs such as self-actualisation are the outcome of a maturational development representing a search for growth and change rather than some kind of drive reduction. Peak experiences, as described by Maslow, are often novel and quite arousing.

Optimal level of arousal

edit

Optimal arousal theory suggested that the increase of cortical arousal was the motive behind all types of stimulus seeking activity (Zuckerman, 1974). It was assumed that the Optimal Level of Arousal (OLA) of HSS is greater than that of LSS, and that HSS function best at a high level of arousal. According to Lee, every person tries to reach an optimal level of arousal from the environment. Too little stimulation causes a person to be bored while too much stimulation causes anxiety which would lead to sensation seeking. Csikszentmihalyi (1998) furthered this theory by looking at how the challenge level of the stimulus and the skill level of the person can affect one's mental state.

 w:Anxietyw:Arousalw:Flow (psychology)w:Overlearningw:Relaxation (psychology)w:Boredomw:Apathyw:Worry
Mental state (Csikszentmihalyi, 1998). (Click on a fragment of the image to go to the appropriate article)

Individual difference theories

edit

Individual difference theories explained differences in personality traits in terms of brain physiology (Zuckerman, 1994):

  • Pavlov (1927/1960) explained temperaments on the basis of innate strengths of excitatory and inhibitory processes in the cortex, the balance between these two types of processes, and their ease of shift from one to another;
  • Eysenck (1967) developed a three-factor ultimate trait system and considered sensation seeking to be a part of extraversion.

However, since 1979, the research on sensation seeking has centred around the biological theory that genetic programs influencing the biochemistry of the central nervous system were the basis of the trait (Zuckerman, 1994). This is because differences in activity of brain catecholamine systems were found to influence arousability of the higher cortical centres (Zuckerman, 1979).

Although environment might determine the particular forms of expression of the trait, the amount of variation in stimulation received during infancy and childhood could also influence the development of the trait (Schaffer, 1971).

The biological approach

edit

The Monoamine Hypothesis

 
The limbic system within the brain.

The most reliable link between biochemical brain events and the sensation seeking trait is that HSS have low levels of monoamine oxidase (MAO) (Zuckerman, 1994). MAO is a limbic system enzyme with a strong genetic determination. It is involved in breaking down brain neurotransmitters such as dopamine and serotonin. Dopamine contributes to the experiences of reward and therefore facilitates approach behaviours (Stellar & Stellar, 1985). Serotonin contributes to a biological inhibition, and to the brains physiological “stop” system, which therefore inhibits approach behaviours (Zuckerman, 1994).

Zuckerman (2006) found that HSS tend to have high levels of dopamine which means that their biochemistry favours approach over inhibition. Furthermore, they also tend to have relatively low levels of serotonin, resulting in their biochemistry failing to inhibit them from risks and new experiences. Low levels of MAO as in HSS have also been linked to paraphilias (Kafka, 1997), tobacco, drug and alcohol use (Zuckerman, 1994).

A hormone responsive to stress, cortisol, is negatively related to HSS, suggesting that low cortisol activity may explain the tendency for HSS to have less symptomatic effects of stress (Balenger, 1983). Gonadal hormones in males are also related to sensation seeking, particularly disinhibition (Zuckerman, 1994). This may account for sex differences in sensation seeking[explain?]. Gonadal hormones are also inversely[explain?] related to socialisation and self-control, which are traits characteristic of LSS (Zuckerman, 1994).

 
A Model of the Neurobiological Sensation Seeking Pathway

Sensation seeking in relation to other dimensions of personality

edit

Costa and McCrae's (1985) NEO Personality Inventory (NEO-PI) was designed around a five-factor model of personality. The five fundamental factors measured by the scale include extraversion, neuroticism, openness to experience, agreeableness and conscientiousness. Costa, McCrae and Dye (1990) found openness to experience to be the strongest correlate to the experience-seeking part of sensation seeking. A slight positive correlation with extraversion was also found. Other sensation seeking factors, such as disinhibition, were found to be negatively correlated with sensation seeking.

Analysis of the NEO-PI factors correlated with sensation confirmed previous findings from other scales measuring similar constructs (Zuckerman, 1994). From this, it can be said that HSS are people who are open to new experiences, particularly in terms of values, actions and fantasies. Furthermore, they tend to be excitement seeking, and tend to easily express and experience their emotions.

Demographic differences

edit

Sensation seeking has been associated to a brain chemistry profile which generates a drive for stimulation and novel experiences. This neuroendocrinological profile is usually found in males more than in females (Zuckerman, 2005). In twin studies, sensation seeking tends to be higher in males than in females. Furthermore, it appears that in utero hormonal influences on later behavioural development influence sensation seeking (Resnick, Gottesman, & McGue, 1993).

Sensation seeking is also correlated with age. Sensation seeking increases between childhood and adolescence; it peaks in adolescence and declines with age (Steinberg, Albert, Cauffman, Banich, Graham, & Woolard, 2008).

As a result, individuals who are sensation seekers are most likely to be males who are adolescents or young adults.

This has also been shown to occur across cultures - between English and American samples (Zuckerman & Eysenck, 1978). However, further research is needed to look at the differences between Eastern and Western cultures.

Risk-taking

edit

Risks related to sensation seeking may be physical, social, financial or legal. HSS are not attracted to these risks but they see sensations and experiences as being worth these risks, while LSS do not (Zuckerman, 1994). Therefore it would be most appropriate to label HSS as "risk accepting" rather than "risk-taking".

Sensation seeking is a primary factor of a risk-taking personality (Zuckerman, 2001). There is a risk-taking continuum, with an antisocial personality at the negative end, prosocial personality at the positive end, and an intermediate point in the centre (Wymer, Self, & Findley, 2008). Sensation seekers at the negative end of the continuum are likely to engage in criminal activities or self destructive behaviours, including drug use, promiscuity, and suicide attempts (Zuckerman, 2000; Slater, 2003). Sensation seekers at the positive end of the continuum may be fire fighters or emergency responders and those in the middle who are neither prosocial nor antisocial may be sensation seekers participating in extreme sports (Zuckerman, 2000).

High sensation seekers are not necessarily at risk for antisocial behaviour. Other biological traits appear to be important in addition to high sensation seeking personality traits (Zuckerman, 2006). For example, for a HSS who is also high in impulsivity, there is a strong relationship with early initiation of sexual activity, number of partners, and use of alcohol or drugs in conjunction with sex (Zuckerman, 2006).

Sports

edit

Sensation seekers are likely to be involved in sports, especially sports requiring risk-taking (Rowland, Fianken, & Harrison, 1986). Extreme sports are sports which include risk-taking as well as skill and athleticism. Extreme sport participants tend to be male sensation seekers (Krisztina, 2006). Several researchers have found extreme sports to be associated with sensation seekers, including skydiving, hang-gliding, rock or ice climbing, whitewater rafting or kayaking, and bungee jumping (Roberts, 1994; Zuckerman, 2000).

  [A video clip featuring sensation seeking behaviour.]

Vocations

edit

Sensation seeking is one of the many traits involved in vocational selection. Some research has found that (Perone, DeWard, & Baron, 1979):

  • Risky vocations attract HSS
  • HSS are more likely to quit their jobs
  • HSS function best in jobs where they are challenged with varying activities
  • LSS function best with predictability and routine

Try this online career test to assess your personality traits to help you decide what job is right for you: Career One Personality Profile Test

Relationships

edit

Comprehending the differences in the way high and low sensation seekers tend behave socially may be useful in understanding and managing one's own social relations.

HSS seem to enjoy and dominate social interactions as it reduces the stress of confinement for them (Zuckerman, 1994). There are multiple notable differences between HSS and LSS that have been supported by research:

Love

edit

Hendrick and Hendrick (1986) found that among the six styles of love, HSS tend to favour the 'Ludus' style, which sees love as a game without strong commitment. LSS were found to favour the 'Pragma' style, which considers the long term potentialities of a partner, more than his or her arousing values. Also, romantic couples tend to have correlated levels of sensation seeking - with sensation seeking being slightly higher for relationship satisfaction in women than in men.

HSS of both sexes have been to have more permissive attitudes towards sex and engage in more varied types of sexual experiences than LSS (Wymer, Self, & Findley, 2008). Zuckerman (1994) found that young married women who are HSS are more hyperarousable and desire sex more often than LSS, however, LSS report more marital and sexual satisfaction. In contrast, Zuckerman found that social deviance and risk taking in sexuality (use of contraception) have not been found to be related to sensation seeking.

Compatibility

edit

HSS tend to be attracted to others that they perceive to be dissimilar from themselves, while LSS prefer the company of those who are similar to themselves (Zuckerman, 1994). Furthermore, HSS value difference and conflict as a source of arousal, while LSS tend to avoid controversies. In love, Franken, Gibson & Mohan, (1990) found that HSS tend to display less relationship satisfaction in terms of liking and loving than LSS. Among married couples, two LSS are more compatible than two HSS.

Leadership

edit

HSS have leadership potential. HSS tend to be original and innovative in open-ended problem solving (Zuckerman, 1994). HSS are also likely to dominate social situations, engage in a great deal of self-disclosure, and encourage self-disclosure from others (Zuckerman, 1994). Sensation seekers are often trend setters because they are socially dominant and are risk takers (Workman & Studak, 2006). Leaders are confronted with risky decisions often and must be willing to accept a level of risk to reach goals (Kowert & Hermann, 1997).

Self-disclosure

edit

Self-disclosure from strangers can be uncomfortable. It may be even more uncomfortable for another to demand self-disclosure from you. HSS seekers tend to engage in more self-disclosure and also encourage more self-disclosure in both casual and close friends (Franken, Gibson & Mohan, 1990).

Body language

edit

HSS are more effective in their non-verbal responses including eye gaze; posture; pattern of vocalisation and are more spontaneous and emotionally expressive (Thornquist, 1991). HSS women tend to gaze more at their partners than LSS, resulting in eliciting gaze and speech from their partners (Thornquist, 1991).

Vicarious experience: Art, media, music, fantasy and humour

edit

Much of the research in this area supported the optimal level of arousal theory which hypothesised that HSS are motivated towards novel experiences and activities in order to avoid the decline in arousal produced by repetition (Lee, 1996).

Please take a moment to reflect upon which item you prefer from the following media pairs.

1 1st Pair

2 2nd Pair

 
 

3 3rd Pair

 
 

NOTE: Answers judged as 'correct' are an indication of high sensation preference; and answers judged as 'incorrect' are an indication of low sensation preference.

HSS tend to have a preference towards complex and ambiguous (as in complex and ambiguous art) and intense (as in rock music) stimuli as they produce more arousal; while LSS tend to prefer, calming, low tension art and music (Zuckerman, 1994).

In general, real life is more arousing than vicarious experience. Research has found that for LSS, vicarious experience may provide the level of arousal they desire (actual experience may be unpleasant). For example, LSS tend to prefer to watch music performances on television while HSS prefer to listen to 'live music' in exciting surroundings (Watson, Anderson, & Schulte, 1977). Furthermore:

  • Based on self-reports and observation, HSS are likely to switch television channels significantly more often than LSS and keep track of multiple programs simultaneously (Schierman & Rowland, 1985).
  • In photographs, television, films and reading, HSS show a greater preference for morbid and sexual themes - LSS tend to find them distasteful and avoid them (Banerjee, Greene, Krcmar, Bagdasarov, & Ruginyte, 2008).
  • HSS are more likely to be found amongst those attending explicit films and horror films (Zuckerman, 1994).

Application

An awareness of the different kinds of media preferences in HSS and LSS has been valuable in the design of media messages aimed at discouraging drug abuse, recruiting volunteers and film marketing (Wymer, Self, & Findley, 2008; Zuckerman, 1994).

  • There is some evidence of a negative relationship[explain?] between daydreaming and sensation seeking (Franken & Rowland, 1990).
  • When HSS do daydream, their fantasies are most likely to be about extravagant hedonic fulfilment in areas such sex, travel and fast driving (Franken & Rowland, 1990).
  • HSS tend to like nonsense humour (where the resolutions are still incongruous or absurd) while LSS like humour in which the punchline neatly ties things up (as in misunderstandings of words or intentions) (Ruch, 1988).

Smoking, drinking, drugs and eating

edit

Smoking

edit

Tobacco is often the first drug to be abused. Although fewer people smoke now than 30 years ago, the relationship between smoking and HSS is still significant - both sexes of HSS smoke more than LSS. This relationship has been found cross-culturally (Zuckerman, 1994).

Drinking and Drugs

edit

Sensation seeking is related to alcohol and drug abuse in adolescence, with early adolescent sensation seeking trait predicting later substance use (Wood, 1995). Marijuana tends to be the first illegal drug used and is favoured among young HSS (Wood, 1995).

Eating

edit

Back (1981) found that HSS in both Western countries and Japan tend to enjoy foods that are spicy and novel to their culture. In contrary, LSS prefer bland, sweet foods. HSS are more willing to try new foods, even those which are very unusual which LSS may regard as 'disgusting'.

Optimal Level of Arousal theory and drugs

Optimal arousal theory predicts that sensation seeking would be positively related to the use of stimulant drugs and negatively related to the use of depressants. However, cumulative evidence suggests that sensation seeking is related to the extent of illegal drug use and variety of drugs used rather than the use of specific classes of drugs (Zuckerman, 1994).

Summary

edit

After reading this chapter you should have a good understanding of where you are on the sensation seeking spectrum and you should be able to start applying this knowledge to various aspects of your life including:

  • vocational preferences and job satisfaction
  • relationships
  • eating habits and food preferences
  • media and art preferences
  • humour
  • social attitudes.

See also

edit

References

edit

Back, K. (1981). Social networks and psychological conditions in diet preferences. Basic and Applied Psychology, 2, 1-9.

Banerjee, S., Greene, K., Krcmar, M., Bagdasarov, Z., & Ruginyte, D. (2008). The role of gender and sensation seeking in film choice. Journal of Media Psychology, 20(3), 97–105. doi: 10.1027/1864-1105.20.3.97

Berlyne, D. (1967). Arousal and reinforcement. In D. Levine (Ed.), Nebraska Symposium on Motivation (Vol. 15), 1-110. Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.

Costa, P. & McCrae, R. (1985). The NEO Personality Inventory Manual. Odessa, FL: Assessment Resources.

Costa, P., McCrae, R., & Dye, D. (1990). Facet Scales for agreeableness and conscientiousness: A revision of the NEO Personality Inventory. Unpublished manuscript. In Zuckerman, 1994.

Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1998). Finding Flow: The Psychology of Engagement With Everyday Life. New York: Basic Books.

Deckers, L. (2005). Motivation: biological, psychological and environmental (2nd Ed.). USA: Pearson: Ally and Bacon.

Fiske, D. & Maddi, S. (1961). Functions of varied experience. Homewood, IL: Dorsey Press.

Franken, R., Gibson, K., & Mohan, P. (1990). Sensation seeking and disclosure. Personality and Individual Differences, 11, 829-832.

Franken, R. & Rowland, G. (1990). Sensation seeking and the tendency to view the world as threatening. Personality and Individual Differences, 13, 31-38.

Freud, S. (1915/1957). Instincts and their vicissitudes. In J. Strachey (Ed.), The Standard Edition of the Complete Psychological Works, 44, 104-140. London: Hogarth Press.

Hull, C. (1943). Principles of Behaviour. New York: Appleton.

Kafka, M. (1997). Hypersexual desire in males: An operational definition and implications for males with paraphilias and paraphilia-related disorders. Archives of Sexual Behaviour, 26, 505-526.

Kowert, P. & Hermann, M. (1997). Daring and caution in foreign policy ranking. Journal of Confiict Resolution, 41(5): 611-637.

Krisztina, M. (2006). The presence of sensation seeking in individuals involved in high-risk sports and high-risk occupations. 2006 World Congress of Performance Analysis of Sport VII, August 23-26, 2006, Szombethely, Hungary; p. 17.

Lee, E. (1996). Arousal Theory and the religiosity-criminality relationship. Contemporary Criminology Theory, 1, 65-84.

Maslow, A. (1954). Motivation and Personality. New York: Harper.

Murray, H. (1938). Explorations in Personality. New York: Oxford University Press.

Perone, M., DeWard, R., & Baron, A. (1979). Satisfaction with real and simulated jobs in relation to personality variables and drug use. Journal of Applied Psychology, 64, 660-668.

Resnick, S., Gottesman, N., & McGue, M. (1993). Sensation seeking in opposite-sex twins: an effect of prenatal hormones? Journal of Behavior Genetics, 23(4): 323-329.

Roberts, P. (1994). Risk the leisure pursuit of danger is a growth industry. Some experts say that courting uncertainly is the only way to define the self. Psychology Today, 24(6): 50.

Rowland, G., Fianken, R., Harrison, K. (1986). Sensation seeking and participating in sporting activities. Journal of Sport Psychology, 8(3): 212-220.

Rowland, G., Fouts, G., & Heatherton, T. (1989). Television viewing and sensation seeking: Uses, preferences and attitudes. Personality and Individual Differences, 10, 1003-1006.

Schaffer, H. (1971). The Growth of Sociability. Baltimore: Penguin Books.

Schierman, M. & Rowland, G. (1985). Sensation seeking and selection of entertainment. Personality and Individual Differences, 6, 599-603.

Slater, M. (2003). Sensation-seeking as a moderator of the effects of peer influences, consistency with personal aspirations, and perceived harm on marijuana and cigarette use among younger adolescents. Substance Use and Misuse, 38(7), 865-880.

Steinberg, L., Albert, D., Cauffman, E., Banich, M., Graham, S., & Woolard, J. (2008). Age differences in sensation seeking and impulsivity as indexed by behavior and self-report. Developmental Psychology, 44(6), 1764-1778. doi: 10.1037/a0012955

Stellar, J. & Stellar, E. (1985). The neurobiology of motivation and reward. New York: Springer-Verlag.

Thornquist, M. (1991). Loving, liking, looking, and sensation seeking. Personality and Individual Differences, 12, 1283-1292.

Walker, L. & Broughton, P. (2009). Motorcycling and Leisure: Understanding the Recreational PTW Rider. Surrey, England: Ashgate Publishing Company.

Watson, C., Anderson, R., & Schulte, D. (1977). Responses of high- and low- deficit patients to exciting, grating and neutral stimuli. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 33, 552-554.

Workman, J. & Studak, C. (2006). Fashion consumers and fashion problem recognition style. International Journal of Consumer Studies, 30(1):75-84.

Wymer, W., Self, D., & Findley, C. (2008). Sensation seekers and civic participation: exploring the influence of sensation seeking and gender on intention to lead and volunteer. International Journal of Nonprofit and Voluntary Sector Marketing, 13, 287-300. doi:10.1002/nvsm.330

Zuckerman, M. (1971). Dimensions of sensation seeking. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 36, 45-52.

Zuckerman, M. (1994). Behavioural Expressions and Biosocial Bases of Sensation Seeking. New York, U.S.A.: Cambridge University Press.

Zuckerman, M. (2000). Are you a risk taker? Psychology Today, 33(6): 52.

Zuckerman, M. (2005). Psychobiology of Personality: Problems in the Behavioural Sciences, 2nd Ed. Cambridge University Press: New York.

Zuckerman, M. (2006). Sensation Seeking and Risky Behavior. American Psychological Association: Washington, DC.

Zuckerman, M., Kolin, I., Price, L., & Zoob, I. (1964). Development of a sensation seeking scale. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 28, 447-482.

Zuckerman, M., Ulrich, R., & McLaughlin, J. (1993). Sensation seeking and reactions to nature paintings. Personality and Individual Differences, 15, 563-576.