Motivation and emotion/Book/2021/Growth needs

Growth needs:
What are growth needs and how do they influence behaviour?

Overview

edit
 
Figure 1. Growth needs drive and guide individuals towards their ultimate potential (Rogers, 1980)

Growth needs emerge once basic physiological (i.e., biological disturbances such as hunger and thirst) and psychological (i.e., mental requirements such as esteem and belonging) needs are satisfied (Maslow, 1943). These growth needs function to drive and guide individuals towards their ultimate potential - a process called self-actualisation (Maslow, 1971).

When an individual pursues growth needs, they are subscribing (consciously or unconsciously) to a growth mindset, which may have a phenomenal influence on behaviour (Mueller & Dweck, 1998). People who pursue growth needs embrace challenges, striving towards personal development through effort (Mueller & Dweck, 1998), which positively affects their psychological health (Miu & Yeager, 2015) and performance (O'Rouke et al., 2014).

This chapter considers psychological theory and research evidence about growth needs, the influence of growth needs on behaviour, and how this information can be helpful for improving one's quality of life (see Figure 1).


Mindset exercise
  1. Reflect on these attitudes:
    • Intelligence is dynamic and something one can greatly improve.
    • Effort drives improvement
    • Intelligence is a generally fixed value determined by genetics/upbringing and can only be marginally improved.
    • Effort implies a lack of talent/ability.
  2. Consider:
    • Which statements resonate with you the most?
    • Do your feelings towards intelligence or effort extend to other areas of your life (e.g., physical ability, career, finances)?
    • Consider that the latter attitudes reflect a fixed mindset, whereas the first two reflect a growth one.
    • After reading through the chapter, return to this section to see if your mindset has shifted.
Focus questions
  • What are growth needs?
  • What is a growth mindset?
  • How does the behaviour of those who pursue growth needs differ from those who don't?

Needs

edit

A need is an essential condition within an individual (Cannon, 1932). Supporting or undermining needs promotes or halts growth, well-being, and life respectively. Needs generate energised, goal-directed, and persistent action (i.e., motivated behaviour; see Figure 2). How they differ from one another is through their influence on the direction of said behaviour (Murray, 1937). Needs can thus be categorised on the basis of the sort of behaviour they motivate. The sources of motivation that encourage eating and drinking are logically distinguishable from the needs that drives mastery of a hobby, for example.

Deficiency needs

edit

Case study It's a typical for Friday night in for Jane, who lives alone after moving away from home for university. Jane logs onto Facebook to check on what her friends are up to and see if anyone is online to chat. In this instance, the deficiency need for relatedness prompted Jane to seek out supportive interpersonal relationships via Facebook (Sheldon et al., 2011; Sheldon & Schuler, 2011).

 
Figure 2. Needs are one component of the three-factor model of internal motivation

Deficiency needs render an individual deprived in some way. This state of deprivation may loosely involve or directly pertain to a plethora of subjects including food, job security, group membership, or social status (Maslow, 1971). In the seminal paper A theory of human motivation, Maslow (1943) posited a wildly popular, graded categorisation of needs: the hierarchy of human needs (see Figure 3). In this model, the more urgently a needs is felt (i.e., the more essential it is for sustaining life), the lower it appears on the pyramid. Physiological needs like oxygen, water, food, sex, sleep, and shelter form the foundation of the structure as crucial requirements for life. These are homeostatic sources of motivation towards the behaviour necessary to avoid bodily damage that would otherwise result in biological decay, pathology, and death (Cannon, 1932).

Each need presents itself sequentially. That is, each level of need generally arises after the previous demand is fulfilled. Table 1 depicts the order in which these deficiency needs typically present, as well as some of their common manifestations. It is important to note that the hierarchies are not strictly separate, but rather are closely interrelated (Maslow, 1954). As such, there is often overlap between the sections. Moreover, while recent research continues to empirically support much of Maslow's needs postulation (Noltemeyer et al., 2020), copious other research finds clear shortcomings (Goebel & Brown, 1981; Alderfer, 1969).

Table 1.

Psychological Deficiency Needs and Respective Examples (adapted from Maslow, 1971).

Deficiency Need Examples
Safety Mental heath, personal security, emotional security, financial security
Love and Belonging Affiliation, family, friendship, intimacy
Esteem Needs Respect, freedom/autonomy, status, self-esteem

Deficiency needs are like vitamins; they are essential because their absence impedes physical and psychological development.

 
Figure 3. Growth needs surface after deficiency needs are satisfied (Maslow, 1943).

Growth needs

edit

Growth needs - or self-actualisation needs - are the impetus and compass which drive and guide individuals towards their ultimate potential (Rogers, 1980). While identifying previous needs is a relatively simple task (i.e., deficiency needs are the motivational force to combat a given deficiency), growth needs are a more obscure concept. In contrast to deficiency need's emergency-like motives, growth needs generate subtle feelings of restlessness, disconnectedness, and yearning (Maslow, 1943). These motives are felt less intensely within the individual but lead to outcomes infinitely more profound: to self-actualisation.

Growth needs motivate individuals towards self-actualisation (see Figure 4). However, without an accurate understanding of exactly what is meant by "self-actualisation", this explanation is insufficient. Self-actualisation can be understood as the overarching control motive that combines the following 14 "meta-needs": wholeness, truth, beauty, spontaneity, justice, simplicity, humour, transcendence, uniqueness, perfection, completion, richness or totality, effortlessness, and autonomy (Maslow,1971).

As displayed by Table 2, self-actualisation is a developmental striving process consisting of two key directions: autonomy and openness to experience. Early humanistic work (Rogers, 1959) posited two concepts pertaining to self-actualisation: the actualising tendency and organismic valuing process, which provide impetus (or "forward thrust of life") and direction respectively. In other words, the actualising tendency fuels a person's desire to grow, and the organismic valuing process provides the person with an innate ability to discern whether certain experiences promote or hinder that growth (Rogers, 1964; Sheldon et al., 2003). In essence, self-actualisation is about leaving behind timidity, defensiveness, and dependence. It necessitates the courageous venturing towards creation, mastery, objective understanding, and self-regulation (Rogers, 1980).

Table 2.

Trajectory and Consequences of Self-actualisation (adapted from Deci & Ryan, 1991; Mittelman, 1991).

Direction Implication
Autonomy Moving away from heteronomy and towards self-sufficiency. Independent regulation of thoughts, feelings and behaviour.
Openness to experience Development of the ability to receive any information (e.g., facts, perceptual information, and/or feelings) without repressing, ignoring, filtering, or distorting it due to any source of bias (e.g., desires, fears, or past experience).
 
Figure 4. Growth needs lead to self-actualisation

One method of identifying growth needs is to note the pathological states that arise from their absence (Maslow, 1971). Table 3 provides some examples of growth needs and the negative psychological effects their absence may have on on a person's affect. Observing pathology is useful because it can simplify the challenging task of identifying growth needs by addressing symptoms like disintegration, dishonesty and humourlessness, rather than the more obscure sentiments of growth, development and potential.

Table 3.

Absence of Growth Needs and Resultant Pathological States (adapted from Maslow, 1971).

Absent need Pathological state
Wholeness Disarray, confusion, overwhelming sense of chaos and disintegration
Aliveness Apathy, disinterest, emotional detachment, humourlessness
Individuality Low self-esteem, negative self-schema, insecurity


 
Quiz

1 Which of the following best exemplifies a growth need?

Hunger
Thirst
The desire to succeed academically
The desire to learn

2 Self-actualisation refers to:

The developmental striving whereby an individual seeks to achieve their ultimate abilities, capacities, and potentialities
The developmental striving whereby an individual makes peace with their shortcomings
A fixed psychological phenomenon whereby individuals overestimate their potential
An out-of-body experience. Individuals believe that their "self" has materially "actualized"

Growth needs on behaviour

edit

Mindsets are mental frameworks used to interpret information and guide cognition (i.e., perception, attention, knowledge formation, working memory, etc.). Growth needs motivate people towards growth behaviours (e.g., skill mastery, challenging tasks, and deep learning). When people pursue this growth, they are nurturing what is called a growth mindset (Mueller & Dweck, 1998).

Mindsets

edit
 
Figure 5. Fixed-minded individuals react poorly to challenging situations compared to growth-seekers.

A common reason people do not pursue growth needs is due to harboring a fixed, not growth mindset (Dweck, 1999). The difference between the two (see Figure 5) comes down to opposing belief sets. Those that possess growth mindsets consider ability as subject to improvement through effort (see Table 4), whereas those with a fixed mindset believe the inverse: that those same qualities are predetermined by some external force (e.g., genetics, environment, opportunity, etc.).

Table 4.

Growth versus Fixed Attitudes Towards Development (adapted from Mueller & Dweck, 1998).

Metric Growth Mindset Fixed Mindset
Intelligence Is readily developed through effort. Is determined by genetics.
Effort Is the main driver of development. Is a last resort for those without natural ability and should be avoided.
Challenge Is an opportunity to learn. Represents an opportunity for incompetence and should be avoided.

A fixed mindset is incredibly limiting. If one believes that they have a set value for intelligence for example, then every interaction with a problem becomes an extrinsically motivated demonstration to prove one's innate ability (Ryan & Deci, 2000). Those with a fixed mindset become consumed by the need to prove themselves and avoid a negative self-concept (Wilson & Ross, 2001). In contrast, the growth-seeking individual sees problems as an opportunity to improve; positive self-concept is sustained through intrinsically motivated behaviour, learning, and reaching their ultimate potential. One study (Moser et al., 2011) comparing children who displayed growth-seeking behaviour with children who displayed the opposite revealed a clear distinction in brain activity between the two groups. While the brains of growth-seekers lit up with electrical activity (see Figure 6), their counterparts saw very little activity, adding neurophysiological evidence to the growing body of empirical support in favor of Dweck's mindset theory.

Behaviour and outcomes

edit

Validation-seeking behaviour (i.e., trying to meet societal conditions of worth) represents another common reason for not pursuing growth needs (Mueller & Dweck, 1998), and leads to worse psychological outcomes. When validation-seeking people pursue external rewards like money, fame, and popularity, they suffer more psychological distress (anxiety, depression, and narcissism) than growth-seekers even if/when they actually attain those things (Kasser, 2002). An unfortunate example of this occurs with professional fashion models. Despite immense wealth, fame, and popularity, they tend to suffer from psychological ill-being, personality maladjustment, and superficial relationships (Meyer et al, 2007). In contrast, growth mindset interventions have been shown to reduce the onset of depression (Miu & Yeager, 2015) and help alleviate depression and anxiety in adolescence (Schleider & Weisz, 2016).

 
Figure 6. Higher electrical activity within the brains of growth-seeking children during problem solving (adapted from Moser et al., 2011)

Another difference between fixed and growth outcomes stems from goal setting strategy. A growth mindset positively correlates with the adoption of mastery goals, which lead individuals to work harder, persist longer, and perform better cognitively (Elliot & Dweck, 1988; Spence, 1983). In contrast, people with fixed mindset are more likely to resort to cheating (Blackwell et al., 2007), coping (e.g., by finding someone worse at the task than them; Nussbaum & Dweck, 2008), or avoidance (Moser et al., 2011). Growth-seekers set goals around effort, process, and learning, while those with a fixed mindset set goals centered around results (Mueller & Dweck, 1998).

Cultivating a growth mindset

edit

Mindsets and patterns of behaviour are malleable (Dweck, 2008). Simply learning this fact can help those with a fixed mindset cultivate a growth one (Yeager et al., 2013). Adolescents who were taught that their intelligence, thoughts, and feelings came from their brains - and could all be changed - more strongly endorsed a growth mindset and increased prosocial behaviour compared to the control group (Yeager et al., 2013). Blackwell et al., (2007) echo these findings; when researches told kids that difficult problems create new neuron connections which make you smarter, they increased grades. This worked especially well for kids who were already struggling (likely fostering fixed mindset; Yeager et al, 2016).

Praise and rewards play a huge role in mindset development (Mueller & Dweck, 1998; Rogers, 1959). Praising wisely (i.e., praising process elements like effort, strategies, focus, perseverance and improvement, rather than talent or intelligence) leads to growth-mindset-fostering students who are hardy and resilient (Gunderson et al., 2013). Similarly, rewarding effort, strategy, and progress, rather than correct answers, leads to more effort, diverse strategies, engagement, and perseverance in student when dealing with difficult math problems (O'Rouke et al., 2014).

Criticisms/limitations

edit

[Provide more detail]

Humanism

edit

Psychological perspectives about growth needs are based on early humanistic psychological theory, which isn't without its flaws. For example, despite its popularity, there's little empirical research supporting Maslow's needs hierarchy (Wahba & Bridwell, 1976), and a great deal that rejects it in its original state (Goebel & Brown, 1981). A two-part model fits the data better, with deficit needs below and self-actualisation needs on the top (Wahba & Bridwell, 1976).

Mindsets

edit

Mindset research has been criticised for being difficult to replicate (Li & Bates, 2019). However, Bryan et al., (2019) note that the research aimed at replication was poorly constructed and that research with sound validity has replicated the findings surrounding mindsets and their benefits (Bryan et al., 2019).

Another common criticism comes from psychologists who believe that growth mindsets as a concept flies in the face of over 100 years of intelligence research. This usually stems from the misinterpretation mindsets are entirely responsible for intelligence. Genetics certainly play a large role in determining intelligence (Byrne, 2009). A growth mindset simply facilitates the realisation of a person's intelligence, which is likely why it works so well when an individual is struggling (Yeager et al., 2016).

An important limitation concerns the small effect size of much of the mindset research. Dweck (2019) addressed this, stating that while psychologists are used to Cohen's guidelines, smaller effect sizes in education research are the norm.

Conclusion

edit

This section presents answers the focus questions and provides the key "take-home" messages, as well as how to implement them in one's life (see Table 5).

Answers to the focus questions
  • Growth needs refer to psychological needs which guide one towards a path of intrinsic development (Maslow, 1971).
  • A growth mindset sees abilities as greatly improvable through effort, and views challenges as an opportunity to do so (Mueller & Dweck, 1998).
  • Those who pursue growth engage in tasks that revolve around enjoyment, skill mastery, learning, and personal development. Those who don't, typically pursue validation or other extrinsic rewards, to their detriment (Kasser, 2002).

Table 5.

Key Points and How to Implement Them

Take-away Implementation
Individuals are motivated to satisfy their needs (Maslow, 1987). Observe the pull of needs in your life and try to be conscious of why you're pursuing certain behaviours (Maslow, 1971). Remember that pursuing deficiency needs is not inherently bad, but essential (Cannon, 1932).
Self-actualisation describes an individual pursuing their ultimate potential (Rogers, 1980). As part of his suggestions for self-actualisation, Maslow (1971) invites one to view life as a series of choices between growth and regression. Choose growth (e.g., sign up for that challenging skill-building class you've been thinking about, rather than binge watch that series you've already seen a thousand times).
The intrinsically motivated pursuit of growth leads to better psychological health (Miu & Yeager, 2015; Schleider & Weisz, 2016) and performance (Spence, 1983;) than pursuing extrinsic rewards, even if one attains those rewards (Kasser, 2002). Set goals centered around activity enjoyment, mastery, and learning. See difficult tasks as an opportunity for growth, not as an indicator of your ability.
How and what we praise and reward influences mindset development (Rogers, 1959; Mueller & Dweck, 1998; O'Rouke et al., 2014). Praise and reward yourself and others for process (i.e., effort, strategies, focus, perseverance and improvement) rather than descriptive characteristics like talent or intelligence.

Finally, remember that your mindsets and patterns of behaviour are malleable (Dweck, 2008). Just by reading this chapter, you've likely begun to cultivate a growth mindset (Yeager et al., 2013).

See also

edit

References

edit
Alderfer, C. P. (1969). An empirical test of a new theory of human needs. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 4, 142-175.

Blackwell, L., Trzesniewski, K., & Dweck, C. (2007). Implicit Theories of Intelligence Predict Achievement Across an Adolescent Transition: A Longitudinal Study and an Intervention. Child Development, 78(1), 246-263.https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.00995.x

Bryan, C., Yeager, D., & O’Brien, J. (2019). Replicator degrees of freedom allow publication of misleading failures to replicate.Proceedings Of The National Academy Of Sciences,116(51), 25535-25545. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1910951116

Byrne, B., Coventry, W. L., Olson, R. K., Samuelsson, S., Corley, R., Willcutt, E. G., Wadsworth, S., & DeFries, J. C. (2009). Genetic and environmental influences on aspects of literacy and language in early childhood: Continuity and change from preschool to grade 2. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 22(3), 219–236. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jneuroling.2008.09.003

Cannon, W. B. (1932). The wisdom of the body. W W Norton & Co.

Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1991). A motivational approach to self: Integration in personality. In R. Dienstbier (Ed.), Nebraska Symposium on Motivation: Perspectives on motivation (Vol. 38, pp. 237–288). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.

Deci, R. M., & Ryan, E. L. (2000). Intrinsic and extrinsic motivations: Classic definitions and new directions. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 25(1), 54-67. https://doi.org/10.1006/ceps.1999.1020

Dweck, C. S. (1999). Self-theories: Their role in motivation, personality, and development. Philadelphia: Psychology Press.

Dweck, C. (2008). Can personality be changed? The role of beliefs in personality and change. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 17, 391-394. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8721.2008.00612.x

Dweck, C. S. (2019). The Choice to Make a Difference. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 14(1), 21–25. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691618804180

Elliott, E., & Dweck, C. (1988). Goals: An approach to motivation and achievement. Journal of Personality And Social Psychology, 54, 5-12. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.54.1.5

Goebel, B. L., & Brown, D. R. (1981). Age differences in motivation related to Maslow’s need hierarchy. Developmental Psychology, 17, 809–815.

Gunderson, E. A., Gripshover, S. J., Romero, C., Dweck, C. S., Goldin-Meadow, S., & Levine, S. C. (2013). Parent praise to 1- to 3-year-olds predicts children's motivational frameworks 5 years later. Child development, 84(5), 1526–1541. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12064

Kasser, T. (2002). The high price of materialism. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Li, Y., & Bates, T. (2019). You can’t change your basic ability, but you work at things, and that’s how we get hard things done: Testing the role of growth mindset on response to setbacks, educational attainment, and cognitive ability. Journal Of Experimental Psychology: General, 148(9), 1640-1655. https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0000669

Maslow, A. H. (1943). A theory of human motivation. Psychological Review, 50(4), 370–396. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0054346

Maslow, A. H. (1954). Motivation and personality. New York: Harper & Row.

Maslow, A. H. (1971). The farther reaches of human nature. New York: Viking Press.

Maslow, A. H. (1987). Motivation and personality (3rd ed.). New York: Harper & Row.

Meyer, B., Enstrom, M. K., Harstveit, M., Bowles, D. P., & Beevers, C. G. (2007). Happiness and despair on the catwalk: Need satisfaction, well-being, and personality adjustment among fashion models. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 2, 2–17. https://doi.org/10.1080/17439760601076635

Mittelman, W. (1991). Maslow’s study of self-actualization: A reinterpretation. Journal of Humanistic Psychology, 31, 114–135.

Miu, A. S., & Yeager, D. S. (2015). Preventing Symptoms of Depression by Teaching Adolescents That People Can Change: Effects of a Brief Incremental Theory of Personality Intervention at 9-Month Follow-Up. Clinical Psychological Science, 3(5), 726–743. https://doi.org/10.1177/2167702614548317

Moser, J., Schroder, H., Heeter, C., Moran, T., & Lee, Y. (2011). Mind Your Errors. Psychological Science, 22(12), 1484-1489. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797611419520

Mueller, C., & Dweck, C. (1998). Praise for intelligence can undermine children's motivation and performance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 33-52. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.75.1.33

Murray, H. A. (1937). Facts which support the concept of need or drive. Journal of Personality, 3, 115–143

Noltemeyer, A., James, A., Bush, K., Bergen, D., Barrios, V., & Patton, J. (2020). The Relationship between Deficiency Needs and Growth Needs: The Continuing Investigation of Maslow’s Theory. Child & Youth Services, 42(1), 24-42. https://doi.org/10.1080/0145935x.2020.1818558

Nussbaum, A., & Dweck, C. (2008). Defensiveness Versus Remediation: Self-Theories and Modes of Self-Esteem Maintenance. Personality And Social Psychology Bulletin, 34(5), 599-612. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167207312960

O'Rourke, E., Haimovitz, K., Ballweber, C., Dweck, C., & Popović, Z. (2014). Brain points. Proceedings Of The SIGCHI Conference On Human Factors In Computing Systems. https://doi.org/10.1145/2556288.2557157

Rogers, C. R. (1959). A theory of therapy, personality, and interpersonal relationships, as developed in the client-centered framework. In S. Koch (Ed.), Psychology: A study of a science (Vol. 3, pp. 184–256).

Rogers, C. R. (1964). Toward a modern approach to values: The valuing process in the mature person. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 68, 160–167.

Rogers, C. R. (1980). A way of being. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.

Schleider JL, Weisz JR. Reducing risk for anxiety and depression in adolescents: Effects of a single-session intervention teaching that personality can change. Behav Res Ther. 2016 Dec;87:170-181. doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2016.09.011.

Sheldon, K. M., Arndt, J., & Houser-Marko, L. I (2003). In search of the organismic valuing process: The human tendency to move towards beneficial goal choices. Journal of Personality, 71, 835–869. doi: https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-6494.7105006

Sheldon, K. M., & Schuler, J. (2011). Wanting, having, and needing: Integrating motive disposition theory and self-determination theory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 101, 1106–1123. doi: https://doi.org/10.1037/a0024952

Sheldon, K. M., Abad, N., & Hinsch, C. (2011). A two-process view of Facebook use and relatedness need-satisfaction: Disconnection drives use, and connection rewards it. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 100, 766–775. doi: https://doi.org/10.1037/2160-4134.1.S.2

Wahba, M. A., & Bridwell, L. G. (1976). Maslow reconsidered: A review of research on the need hierarchy theory. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 15, 212–240.

Wilson, A. E., & Ross, M. (2001). From chump to champ: People's appraisals of their earlier and present selves. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80(4), 572–584. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.80.4.572

Yeager, D. S., Romero, C., Paunesku, D., Hulleman, C. S., Schneider, B., Hinojosa, C., . . . Dweck, C. S. (2016). Using design thinking to improve psychological interventions: The case of the growth mindset during the transition to high school. Journal of Educational Psychology, 108, 374–391. https://doi.org/10.1037/edu0000098

Yeager, D., Trzesniewski, K., & Dweck, C. (2012). An Implicit Theories of Personality Intervention Reduces Adolescent Aggression in Response to Victimization and Exclusion. Child Development, 84(3), 970-988. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12003

edit